Keres language

(Redirected from Western Keres language)

Keres (/ˈkrs/),[2] also Keresan (/ˈkɛrəsən/), is a Native American language, spoken by the Keres Pueblo people in New Mexico. Depending on the analysis, Keres is considered a small language family or a language isolate with several dialects. If it is considered a language isolate, it would be the most widely spoken language isolate within the borders of the United States. The varieties of each of the seven Keres pueblos are mutually intelligible with its closest neighbors. There are significant differences between the Western and Eastern groups, which are sometimes counted as separate languages.

Keresan
Native toUnited States
RegionNew Mexico
EthnicityKeres
Native speakers
13,190 (2013)[1]
Dialects
  • East Keres
  • West Keres
Language codes
ISO 639-3Either:
kee – Eastern
kjq – Western
Glottologkere1287
ELP
Pre-contact distribution of Keresan languages
Acoma-Laguna is classified as Definitely Endangered by the UNESCO Atlas of the World's Languages in Danger

Family division

In 2007, there was an estimate total of 10,670 speakers.[3]

  • Keres
    • Eastern Keres: total of 4,580 speakers (1990 census)
    • Western Keres: total of 3,391 speakers (1990 census)

Genetic relationships

Keres is now considered a language isolate. In the past, Edward Sapir grouped it together with a Hokan–Siouan stock. Morris Swadesh suggested a connection with Wichita. Joseph Greenberg grouped Keres with Siouan, Yuchi, Caddoan, and Iroquoian in a superstock called Keresiouan. None of these proposals has been validated by subsequent linguistic research.

Phonology

Keresan has between 42 and 45 consonant sounds, and around 40 vowel sounds, adding up to a total of about 85 phonemes, depending on the analysis and the language variety. Based on the classification in the World Atlas of Language Structures, Keres is a language with a large consonant inventory.

The great number of consonants relates to the three-way distinction between voiceless, aspirated and ejective consonants (e.g. /t tʰ tʼ/), and to the larger than average[4] number of fricatives (i.e. /s sʼ ʂ ʂʼ ʃ ʃʼ h/) and affricates, the latter also showing the three-way distinction found in stops.

The large number of vowels derives from a distinction made between long and short vowels (e.g. /e eː/), as well as from the presence of tones and voicelessness. Thus, a single vowel quality may occur with seven distinct realizations: /é è e̥ éː èː êː ěː/, all of which are used to distinguish words in the language.

Consonants

The chart below contains the consonants of the proto-Keresan (or pre-Keresan) from Miller & Davis (1963) based on a comparison of Acoma, Santa Ana, and Santo Domingo, as well as other features of the dialects compiled from The Language of Santa Ana Pueblo (1964), Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics (1987), and The Phonemes of Keresan (1946), and the Grammar of Laguna Keres (2005).[5][6][7][8]

LabialAlveolarPalatalRetroflexVelarGlottal
Nasalvoicedmnɲ
glottalizedɲˀ
Plosivevoicelessptckʔ
aspirated
ejective
Affricatevoicelessts
aspiratedtsʰtʃʰtʂʰ
ejectivetsʼtʃʼtʂʼ
Fricativevoicelesssʃʂh
ejectiveʃʼʂʼ
Approximantvoicedwɾj
glottalizedɾˀ

Vowels

Keresan vowels have a phonemic distinction in duration: all vowels can be long or short. Additionally, short vowels can also be voiceless. The vowel chart below contains the vowel phonemes and allophones from the information of the Keresan languages combined from The Language of Santa Ana Pueblo (1964),[5] The Phonemes of Keresan (1946),[7] and Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics (1987).[6]

LongShort
PhonemicPhoneticPhonemicPhoneticVoiceless
Close/iː/[i]/i/[i ɪ][ɪ̥]
Mid-front/eː/[eː]/e/[e ɛ æ][e̥]
Mid-central/ɨː/[əː ɨː]/ɨ/[ə ɨ ɤ][ɨ̥]
Open/ɑː/[aː ɑː]/ɑ/[a ɑ][ḁ]
Back-close/oː/[oː]/o/[o][o̥]
/uː/[uː]/u/[u ʊ o][ʊ̥]

Notes:

  • Western Keres does not have phonemic /oː/ or /o/, though both vowels may occur phonetically.[8] Eastern Keres words containing /o/ show /au/ in Western Keres.[9] For instance, the first vowel in the word-sentence Sraúka̠cha̠ – “I see you”:
    • Kotyit Keres: [ʂóːkʰɑ̥tʃʰɑ̥]
    • Kʼawaika Keres: [ʂɑ̌ukʰɑ̥tʃʰɑ̥]

Voiceless vowels

All Keresan short vowels may be devoiced in certain positions. The phonemic status of these vowels is controversial.[8] Maring (1967) considers them to be phonemes of Áákʼu Keres, whereas other authors disagree. There are phonetic grounds for vowel devoicing based on the environment they occur, for instance word-finally, but there are also exceptions. Vowels in final position are nearly always voiceless and medial vowels occurring between voiced consonants, after nasals and ejectives are nearly always voiced.[10]

  • Word-final devoicing: [pɑ̌ːkʊ̥] because
  • Word-medial devoicing: [ʔìpʰi̥ʃɑ́] white paint

Tones

Acoma Keres has four lexical tones: high, low, falling and rising.[10] Falling and rising tones only occur in long vowels and voiceless vowels bear no tones:

Tonesexamplestranslation
High[tɨ́j], [áwáʔáwá]here, matrilineal uncle
Low[mùːtètsá]young boy
Rising[pɑ̌ːkʊ̥]because
Falling[ʔêː], [hêːk'a]and, whole part

Syllable structure

Most Keresan syllables take a CV(V) shape.[8] The maximal syllable structure is CCVVC and the minimal syllable is CV. In native Keresan words, only a glottal stop /ʔ/ ⟨ʼ⟩ can close a syllable, but some loanwords from Spanish have syllables that end in a consonant, mostly a nasal (i.e. /m n/ but words containing these sequences are rare in the language.[11]

Syllable typeexamplestranslation
CV[sʼà], [ʔɪ]shv́vI have it, left
CVV[mùː]dedza, a[táù]shiyoung boy, cooking pot
CCV[ʃkʰí]srátsʼaI'm not fat
CCVV[ʃtùː]srabluejay
CVCí[miʔ], [kùm]banêeruexpression of fear, workmate (Spanish "compañero")

Due to extensive vowel devoicing, several Keresan words may be perceived as ending in consonants or even containing consonant clusters.

  • Word-internal cluster: yʼâakạ srûunị ‘stomach’ /jˀɑ̂ːkḁʂûːni/ > [jɑ̂ːkḁʂûːni] ~ [jɑ̂ːûːni]
  • Word-final coda: úwàakạ ‘baby’; /úwɑ̀ːkḁ/ > [úwɑ̀ːkʰḁ] ~ [úwɑ̀ː]

Phonotactics

The only sequence of consonants (i.e. consonant cluster) that occurs in native Keresan words is a sequence of a fricative /ʃ ʂ/ and a stop or affricate. Clusters are restricted to beginnings of syllables (i.e. the syllable onset). When the alveolo-palatal consonant /ʃ/ occurs as C1, it combines with alveolar and palatal C2, whereas the retroflex alveolar /ʂ/ precedes bilabial and velar C2s, which suggest a complementary distribution. Consonant clusters may occur both word-initially and word-medially.[9]

C1/C2BilabialAlveolarVelarPostalveolar
/p//pʰ//pʼ//t//tʰ//tʼ//k//kʰ//kʼ//tʃ//tʃʰ//tʃʼ/
/ʃ//ʃtáʊ̯rákʊ̥/

shdáurákụ

'frog, toad'

/ʃtʰéràʃtʼíká/

shtérashtʼígá

'cricket'

/ʃtʼìcɑ̀ːtʰɪ̥ʃɪ̥/

shtʼidyàatịshị

'plot of land'

/ʃtʃɨ/

shjv

'upward'

/ʃtʃʰúmúná/

shchúmúmá

'wasp'

/ʃtʃʼísḁ/

shchʼísạ

'six'

/ʂ//ʂpúːná/

srbúuná

'water jug'

/ʂpʰɑ̀ːtʼi/

srpàat'i

'mockingbird'

/ʂpʼeruru/

srpʼeruru

'it's full'

/ʂkɑ́ʂkɑ́ʊ̯kʼa/

srgásrgáukʼa

'quail'

/ʂkʰɨ́tútsʰɪ̥/

srkv́dútsị

'mound, hill'

/ʂkʼàpɪ́hɪ́/

srkʼabíhí

'female in-law'

Orthography

Traditional Keresan beliefs postulate that Keres is a sacred language that must exist only in its spoken form.[12] The language's religious connotation and years of persecution of Pueblo religion by European colonizers may also explain why no unified orthographic convention exists for Keresan. However, a practical spelling system has been developed for Laguna (Kʼawaika)[8] and more recently for Acoma (Áakʼu) Keres,[13] both of which are remarkably consistent.

In the Keres spelling system, each symbol represents a single phoneme. The letters ⟨c q z f⟩ and sometimes also ⟨v⟩ are not used. Digraphs represent both palatal consonants (written using a sequence of C and ⟨y⟩), and retroflex consonants, which are represented using a sequence of C and the letter ⟨r⟩. These graphemes used for writing Western Keres are shown between ⟨...⟩ below.

Consonant symbols

LabialAlveolarPalatalRetroflexVelarGlottal
Nasalvoiced⟨m⟩⟨n⟩⟨ny⟩
glottalized⟨mʼ⟩⟨nʼ⟩⟨nyʼ⟩
Plosivevoiceless⟨b⟩⟨d⟩⟨dy⟩⟨g⟩⟨ʼ⟩
aspirated⟨p⟩⟨t⟩⟨ty⟩⟨k⟩
ejective⟨pʼ⟩⟨tʼ⟩⟨tyʼ⟩⟨kʼ⟩
Affricatevoiceless⟨dz⟩⟨j⟩⟨dr⟩
aspirated⟨ts⟩⟨ch⟩⟨tr⟩
ejective⟨tsʼ⟩⟨chʼ⟩⟨trʼ⟩
Fricativevoiceless⟨s⟩⟨sh⟩⟨sr⟩⟨h⟩
ejective⟨sʼ⟩⟨shʼ⟩⟨srʼ⟩
Approximantvoiced⟨w⟩⟨r⟩⟨y⟩
glottalized⟨wʼ⟩⟨rʼ⟩⟨yʼ⟩

Signage at Acoma Pueblo

Signs at Acoma Pueblo sometimes use special diacritics for ejective consonants that differ from the symbols above, as shown in the table:

Signage at Acoma Pueblo
General⟨pʼ⟩⟨tʼ⟩⟨kʼ⟩⟨sʼ⟩⟨tsʼ⟩⟨mʼ⟩⟨wʼ⟩⟨yʼ⟩⟨nʼ shʼ srʼ tyʼ⟩
Acoma signage⟨ṕ⟩⟨t́⟩⟨ḱ⟩⟨ś⟩⟨tś⟩⟨ḿ⟩⟨ẃ⟩⟨ý⟩?

Vowel symbols

Vowel sounds are represented straightforwardly in the existing spellings for Keresan. Each vowel sound is written using a unique letter or digraph (for long vowels and diphthongs). However, there are two competing representations for the vowel /ɨ/. Some versions simply use the IPA ⟨ɨ⟩ whereas others use the letter ⟨v⟩ (the sound /v/ as in veal does not occur in Keresan). Voiceless vowels have also been represented in two ways; either underlined or with a dot below (see table).

Long vowelsShort vowelsVoiceless vowels
PhonemeGraphemePhonemeGraphemePhonemeGrapheme
/ iː /⟨ii⟩/ i /⟨i⟩/ ɪ̥ /⟨i̱⟩ or ⟨ị⟩
/ eː /⟨ee⟩/ e /⟨e⟩/ e̥ /⟨e̱⟩ or ⟨ẹ⟩
/ ɨː /⟨ɨɨ⟩ or ⟨vv⟩/ ɨ /⟨ɨ⟩ or ⟨v⟩/ ɨ̥ /⟨ɨ̱⟩ or ⟨ṿ⟩
/ ɑː /⟨aa⟩/ ɑ /⟨a⟩/ ḁ /⟨a̱⟩ or ⟨ạ⟩
/ oː /⟨oo⟩/ o /⟨o⟩/ o̥ /⟨o̱⟩ or ⟨ọ⟩
/ uː /⟨uu⟩/ u /⟨u⟩/ ʊ̥ /⟨u̱⟩ or ⟨ụ⟩

Diacritics for tone

Tone may or may not be represented in the orthography of Keresan. When represented, four diacritics may be used above the vowel. Unlike the system used for Navajo, diacritics for tone are not repeated in long vowels.

High toneLow toneRising toneFalling tone
Long Vowel⟨áa⟩, ⟨úu⟩⟨àa⟩, ⟨ùu⟩ or unmarked⟨ǎa⟩, ⟨ǔu⟩ or ⟨aá⟩, ⟨uú⟩⟨âa⟩, ⟨ûu⟩ or ⟨aà⟩, ⟨uù⟩
Short Vowel⟨á⟩, ⟨ú⟩⟨à⟩, ⟨ù⟩ or unmarked-

Keres alphabet and alphabetical order

Although Keresan is not normally written, there exists only one dictionary of the language in which words are listed in any given order. In this dictionary of Western Keres, digraphs count as single letters, although ejective consonants are not listed separately; occurring after their non-ejective counterparts. The glottal stop ⟨ʼ⟩ and long vowels (e.g. ⟨aa ee ii⟩ etc.) are not treated as separate letters.

Alphabetical order in the Acoma Keres Audio Dictionary
ABCHCHʼDDRDYDZEGHIJKMNNYNYʼP
RSSHSHʼSRSRʼTTRTRʼTSTSʼTYTYʼUWY

Sample texts

Orthography marking tone

Woodpecker and Coyote[9]
⟨Ái dítʼîishu srbígà kʼánâaya dyáʼâʼu. Shʼée srbígà ái dyěitsị ái náyáa shdyɨ dyáʼa.⟩
/ ɑ́ì títʼîːʃù ʂpíkɑ̀ kʼɑ́nɑ̂ːjɑ̀ cɑ́ʔɑ̂ʔù | ʃʼéː ʂpíkɑ̀ ɑ́ì cěǐtsʰi̥ ɑ́ì nɑ́jɑ́ː ʃcɨ̀ cɑ́ʔɑ̀ /

Orthography without tone marking

Boas text[8]
Baanaʼa, egu kauʼseeʼe, atsi sʼaama-ee srayutse.

Morphosyntax

Keresan is a split-ergative language in which verbs denoting states (i.e. stative verbs) behave differently from those indexing actions, especially in terms of the person affixes they take. This system of argument marking is based on a split-intransitive pattern, in which subjects are marked differently if they are perceived as actors than from when they are perceived as undergoers of the action being described.

The morphology of Keresan is mostly prefixing, although suffixes and reduplication also occur.[9] Keresan distinguishes nouns, verbs, numerals and particles as word classes. Nouns in Keresan do not normally distinguish case or number, but they can be inflected for possession, with distinct constructions for alienable and inalienable possession. Other than possession, Keresan nouns show no comprehensive noun classes.

Word order

Keresan is a verb-final language, though word order is rather flexible.[9][8]

Laguna Keres:[8]

John

J.

John

subject

Bill

B.

Bill

object

gukacha

g-Ø-ukacha

3S-3S-see

verb

John Bill gukacha

J. B. g-Ø-ukacha

John Bill 3S-3S-see

subject object verb

'John saw Bill.'

Negation

Negation is doubly marked in Keresan. In addition to the adverb dzaadi, verbs index negation through a suffix (e.g. -u).

  • Gukacha 'S/he saw her/him'
  • Dzaadi gukachau 'S/he didn't see her/him'

Verbal morphology

The verb is a central grammatical category in Keres, conveying the most information about events in communicative acts.[8][9][10] Through its morphemes, Keresan verbs code not only person and number of the initiator of the action (e.g. “Tammy drinks decaf”) as is common in Indo-European languages, but also how the initiator is implicated in the action. For instance, the three verbs that describe Tammy's actions in “Tammy kicked the ball” vs. “Tammy jumped” vs. “Tammy sneezed” require different levels of effort from Tammy, that is when kicking vs. jumping vs. sneezing.

Additionally, the person and number of the undergoer of the action are all coded on the verb (e.g. the word gukacha means “S/he sees her/him”, a full sentence in English). The ways the speaker assesses the action (i.e. evidentiality, as in “I think Tammy arrived from class” vs. “Tammy arrived from class”). Finally, the internal temporal structure of the action (i.e. aspect, as in “Tammy was sneezing in class” vs. “Tammy sneezed in class”) is also coded in Keresan verbs.

According to Maring (1967), the Keresan verb is organized around the following grammatical categories (pp. 39–40)[10]

  • Subject/Object relations
    • Subject of intransitive verbs: marked by a prefix that distinguishes 3-4 persons in the singular (see below).
    • Subject of transitive verbs: marked by a prefix that distinguishes 3-4 persons in the singular (see below).
    • Object of transitive verbs: marked by a prefix that combines with the subject prefix, or by a suffix
  • Number relations
    • Singular: usually marked by a prefix
    • Dual: can be marked by a prefix, partial reduplication or by suffixes
    • Plural: can be marked by a prefix, partial reduplication, by suffixes or by suppletive stem forms (i.e. singular and plural forms are not related etymologically)
  • Temporal relations
    • Future: is marked on the verb by a series of prefixes that also encode number
  • Modality relations
  • Voice relations
  • Aspect
    • Imperfective
    • Inceptive
    • Repetitive
    • Continuative
    • Habitual
    • Inchoative
    • Perfective

The verbal prefix

In Keres, the verbal prefix carries information from five different grammatical categories: argument role, modality, polarity,[8] person and number. That is, a single Keresan verb prefix codes who initiated the action and how implicated that entity is (the subject/case), whom underwent the effects of the action (the direct object), the speaker's assessment of the action (the modality)[14] and whether it occurred or not (polarity). On the other hand, information about when the action took place (i.e. tense) is expressed elsewhere in a clause, mostly by adverbs.[9]

Number

Keresan verbs distinguish three numbers: singular, dual (two entities) and plural (more than two entities); and four persons: first (the speaker), second (the hearer), third (a known, definite or salient entity being talked about) and fourth (a non-salient, unknown or indefinite entity being talked about, also known as obviative) persons. The plural and dual forms are often marked by reduplication of part of the stem (gukacha ‘s/he saw it’ vs guʼukacha ‘the two of them saw it’).

Argument role

Languages encode two main types of actions: those in which the main participant initiates an action that produces change in an object (e.g. kick a ball, buy a gift, cook a dish, read a book); and those in which the action produces no (perceived) change in the world or that have no object (sneezing, breathing, growing, diving, etc.).[15] Actions that take an object are encoded by transitive verbs, whereas those that take no object are expressed via intransitive verbs.

Intransitive verbs

In Indo-European languages like English, all intransitive verbs behave similarly (‘They sneeze/breathe/dive/think’/etc.). In Keresan, actions that take no object are conceptualized in two distinct ways depending on how the initiator of the action is implicated. More active-like intransitive verbs (e.g. ‘to sneeze’) are coded through one set of morphemes, whereas actions conceptualized as involving the initiator at a lesser degree (e.g. ‘to believe’) are coded using a separate set of prefixes.

Degrees of involvement of the initiator in Keres[8]
ActionsIntransitive verb type
Moreto write (-dyàatra), to steal as a thief (-chʼáwʼa), to have diarrhea (-ushchʼi),

to leave (-mi), to whistle (-srbiitsa), to sweat (-shdyuwàan’i)

Active
Lessto believe (-hima), to be born (-dyá), to sleep (-bái),

to be afraid (-tyishu), to forget (-dyúmidruwi)

Inactive

Ideas expressed in Indo-European languages with adjectives are most often encoded by verbs in Keresan. That is, in Keresan one express the idea in the sentence ‘He is selfish’ by saying something along the lines of ‘He selfishes’. In such “actions”, the entity that is characterized by them is not implicated in the action directly (i.e. it's beyond their control), and thus belong in the Inactive intransitive category. The different sets of prefixes are shown below:

Intransitive Prefixes by Verb Type
Active intransiveInactive intransitive
PrefixExamplePrefixExample
Firsts(i)-sudyàatraI writesrk-srkuhimaI believe
Secondsr-srúuchʼáwʼayou stealkɨdr-kɨdrâidyáyou were born
Thirdk-kashdyuwàanʼis/he sweatsdz-dzíibáihe is sleeping
Transitive verbs
Transitive verb - Indicative mood (-ukạchạ 'to see')
Direct object
Singular
SubjectFirst ('me’)Second (‘you’)Third (‘her’/‘him’)Fourth
First

(‘I’)

-srà-ukạchạsì-ukạchạ-
I see youI see her/him
Second

(‘you’)

dyù-ukạchạ-srù-ukạchạ
you see meyou see her/him
Third

(‘she’/‘he’)

srgù-ukạchạkudrù -ukạchạg-ukạchạgù-ukạchạ
s/he sees mes/he sees yous/he sees her/hims/he sees something
Fourth

(‘one’)

-dzì-ukạchạ-
one sees it

Aspect

Aspect in Keresan is signalled by suffixes.

-ajanu 'to rain'
kájániit rains
káajániit is raining
kájásɨit keeps raining
káajatúit rained

Time (tense) adverbials

The category of tense is expressed in Keresan via adverbs that indicate when the action about which one is speaking took place.

Time adverbials in Acoma Keres[10]
PastFuture
tsikʼínumalong agokúsratonight
hámaonce, formerlynachámatomorrow
súwayesterdaynaháayashiday after tomorrow

Lexicon

New words are coined through a number of roots that are combined to pre-existing ones. Compounding is a common strategy for word building, although derivation also occurs.

Numerals

The Keresan numeral system is a base 10 system. Numerals 11–19, as well as those between the multiple of tens, are formed by adding the word kʼátsi (/ kʼátsʰɪ / 'ten') followed by the word dzidra (/tsɪtʂa/ 'more'). Numerals 20 and above are formed by adding a multiplicative adverb (-wa or -ya) to the base number and the word kʼátsi.[8]

Western Keres
1ísrkʼé11kʼátsi-írskʼá-dzidra21dyúya-kʼátsi-íisrkʼé-dzidra
2dyúuwʼée12kʼátsi-dyú-dzidra22dyúya-kʼátsi-dyú-dzidra
3chameʼée13kʼátsi-chami-dzidra30chamiya-kʼátsi
4dyáana14kʼátsi-dyáana-dzidra40dyáanawa-kʼátsi
5táam'a15kʼátsi-táamʼa-dzidra50táamʼawa-kʼátsi
6shʼísa16kʼátsi-shchʼísa-dzidra60shchʼísawa-kʼátsi
7mʼáiʼdyàana17kʼátsi-mʼáidyana-dzidra70mʼáidyanawa-kʼátsi
8kukʼúmishu18kʼátsi-kukʼúmishu-dzidra80kukʼúmishuwa-kʼátsi
9máyúkʼu19kʼátsi-máiyúkʼa-dzidra90máiyúkʼuwa-kʼátsi
10kʼátsi20dyúwa-kʼátsi100kʼádzawa-kʼátsi

Loanwords from Spanish

European colonizers arriving in the Southwest US brought with them material culture and concepts that were unknown to the peoples living in the area. Words for the new ideas introduced by Spaniards were often borrowed into Keres directly from Early Modern Spanish, and a large number of these persists in Modern Keresan.[11]

Semantic domainModern Western KeresModern SpanishEnglish translation
Household itemskamárîita, kuchâaru, kujûuna, méesa, mendâan, kuwêetacamarita, cuchara, colchón, mesa, ventana, cubeta (Mexico)bed, spoon, mattress, table, window (glass), bucket
Social structuregumbanêerụ, rái, murâatụ, merigâanạ, kumanirá, ninêerucompañero, rey, mulato, americano(a), comunidad, dineroworkmate, king, black person, white person, community house, money
Foodgéesu, arûusị, kawé, kurántụ, mantạgîiyụ, mandêegạqueso, arroz, café, cilantro, mantequilla, mantecacheese, rice, coffee, cilantro, butter, lard/butter
Animal husbandrykawâayu, kanêeru, kujíinu, kurá, dûura, wáakạshịcaballo, carnero, cochino, corral, toro, vacahorse, sheep, pen/corral, bull, cow
Religious conceptsmíisa, Háasus Kuríistị, nachạwêena, guréesimamisa, Jesús Cristo, Noche Buena, Cuaresmamass, Jesus Christ, Christmas, Lent
Days of the weektamîikụ, rûunishị, mâatịsị, mérikụsị, sruwêewesị, yêenịsị, sâawarudomingo, lunes, martes, miércoles, jueves, viernes, sábadoSunday, Monday, Tuesday, Wednesday, Thursday, Friday, Saturday

Proto-language

Proto-Keresan
Reconstruction ofKeresan languages

Selected Proto-Keresan reconstructions of plants, animals, and toponyms by Miller and Davis (1963):[16]

no.glossProto-Keresan
10wheat*ʔáṣánɪ
17centipede*ʔíʔìˑdʸawa
19cholla cactus*ʔiˑbánɪ
27porcupine*ʔiˑṣ̍á
45toad*bêˑrak̠ᴀ
63turkey*cinᴀ
64fox*cúsk̠ɪ
71locust*c̍íˑga
72Zia Pueblo*c̍íˑy̍á
78kiva*c̆ídʸá
83medicine man*č̇áyâˑni
84hawk*č̇ɨ́ˑríga
85horned toad*dabínᴜsk̠ᴀ
87Santa Ana Pueblo*dámáyá
88squash*dâˑni
91corn husk*díˑskámí
93dog*díyᴀ
98bobcat*dʸáˑdʸᴜ
101deer*dʸán̍é
104gourd*dʸáˑwí
105piñon pine*dʸèic̠ɪ
108elk*dʸɨ́ˑṣᴀ
110badger*dʸúˑbí
112beans*gánami
114seed*gáwɪc̠ɪ
119bear*gúháyᴀ
124yucca*háʔásc̐á
127oak*ha̍ˑbánɪ
137pine tree*hâˑniˑ
147Jemez Pueblo*héˑmíšíˑ-cɪ, *héˑmíšíˑ-zé
149turtle*héyᴀdʸɪ
157willow*híẓᵻsk̍áwa
158dove*húˑʔùˑga
161yucca fruit*hùˑsk̍ani
169antelope*kɨ́ˑc̠ɪ
175wolf*k̍ákana
176spider*k̍ámᴀsk̠ᵻ
198mountain lion*mûˑk̍aiẓᴀ
200buffalo*múšêiẓᴀ
201soapweed*múšɪ
213hummingbird*m̍îˑzᴀ
225prairie dog*nɨ́t̠ɪ
232bedbug*peséc̍uru
239salamander*p̍águra
241rabbit*rèˑdʸᴀ
246woodpecker*sbíga
247chicken*sbíˑná
251meadowlark*sc̐áˑná
254grasshopper*sc̐ár̍ɪ
260crow*sc̐ɨ́r̍á
262wild honey*sc̐úmᵻ
264mosquito*sc̐úy̍úˑná
274ant*síˑʔí
275squirrel*síˑdʸᴀ
279mouse*síyan̍ᵻ
282bighorn sheep*skàˑsk̠ᴜ
286bullsnake*sk̍áʔáˑdʸᴜ
287fish*sk̍àˑšᵻ
291peas*sk̍úrúˑná
293dwarf corn*spíníní
306parrot*šâˑwit̠ᴀ
307flea, louse*šínaˑ
309goose*šúˑdá
318blue jay*ṣúisɪ
319snake*ṣûˑwiˑ
342abalone shell*w̍a̍ˑbɨ́nɪ
347duck*w̍âˑyuṣᴀ
354corn silk*yábášɪ
355corn*yáˑčínɪ
356mesquite*yêˑt̠ᴜ
357worm*yúʔúbɨ́
369corn cob*y̍úˑskúm̍á

Keres was one of the seven languages sung in the Coca-Cola "It's Beautiful" commercial during the 2014 Super Bowl featuring "America the Beautiful".[17]

See also

References

Bibliography